Some of the regional varieties of the Spanish language are quite divergent from one another, especially in pronunciation and vocabulary, and less so in grammar.
While all Spanish dialects adhere to approximately the same written standard, all spoken varieties differ from the written variety, to different degrees. There are differences between European Spanish (also called Peninsular Spanish) and the Spanish of the Americas, as well as many different dialect areas both within Spain and within Hispanic America.
Prominent differences of pronunciation among dialects of Spanish include:
- the maintenance or loss of distinction between the phonemes /?/ and /s/ (distinción vs. seseo and ceceo);
- the maintenance or loss of distinction between phonemes represented orthographically by ll and y (yeísmo);
- the maintenance of syllable-final [s] vs. its weakening to [h] (called aspiration, or more precisely debuccalization), or its loss; and
- the tendency, in areas of central Mexico and of the Andean highlands, to reduction (especially devoicing), or loss, of unstressed vowels, mainly when they are in contact with voiceless consonants.
Among grammatical features, the most prominent variation among dialects is in the use of the second-person pronouns. In Hispanic America the only second-person plural pronoun, for both formal and informal treatment, is ustedes, while in most of Spain the informal second-person plural pronoun is vosotros with ustedes used only in the formal treatment. For the second-person singular familiar pronoun, some Hispanic America dialects use tú (and its associated verb forms), while others use either vos (see voseo) or both tú and vos (which, together with usted, can make for a possible three-tiered distinction of formalities).
There are significant differences in vocabulary among regional varieties of Spanish, particularly in the domains of food products, everyday objects, and clothes; and many Hispanic American varieties show considerable lexical influence from Native American languages.
Video Spanish dialects and varieties
Sets of variants
In a broad sense, Hispanic American Spanish can be grouped into:
- Mexican
- Central American
- Caribbean (Cuba, Venezuela, Puerto Rico, Dominican Republic, Panama, Caribbean Colombia and Caribbean Mexico).
- Andean-Pacific (Colombia, Peru, Ecuador, West Bolivian, and Andean Venezuela).
- Rioplatense (Argentina, Uruguay, East Bolivian and Paraguay)
- Chilean (Chile, Cuyo)
Old World varieties are:
- Northern Peninsular (Asturias, Castilla y León, Cantabria, Basque country, Navarre, Aragón, Rioja, Provinces of Guadalajara and Cuenca)
- Central-Southern Peninsular (Madrid, Toledo, La Mancha)
- Southern Peninsular (Andalusia, Extremadura, and Murcia)
- Canarian (Canary Islands)
- the ebbing Philippine Spanish (Philippines)
The non-native Spanish in Equatorial Guinea and Western Sahara (formerly Spanish Sahara) has been influenced mainly by varieties from Spain.
The Spanish spoken in Gibraltar is essentially not different from the neighboring areas in Spain, except for code-switching with English.
Maps Spanish dialects and varieties
Pronunciation
Distinción vs. seseo and ceceo
The distinction between the historical phonemes /s/ and /?/ is maintained in northern Spain (in all positions) and in south-central Spain (only in syllable onset), while the two phonemes are merged in Hispanic America, the Canary Islands, and much of Andalusia. The maintenance of phonemic contrast is called distinción in Spanish. The merged phoneme is typically realized as [s], though in parts of southern Andalusia the realization is closer to [?]; in Spain this latter variation is called ceceo and the former one seseo.
In dialects with seseo the words casa ("house") and caza ("hunt") are pronounced as homophones (generally ['kasa]), whereas in dialects with distinción they are pronounced differently (as ['kasa] and ['ka?a] respectively). The symbol [s] stands for a voiceless sibilant like the s of English sick, while [?] represents a voiceless interdental fricative like the th of English thick.
In some cases where the phonemic merger would render words homophonic in Hispanic America, one member of the pair is frequently replaced by a synonym or derived form--e.g. caza replaced by cacería, or cocer ("to boil"), homophonic with coser ("to sew"), replaced by cocinar. For more on seseo, see González-Bueno.
Yeísmo
Traditionally Spanish had a phonemic distinction between /?/ (a palatal lateral approximant, written ll) and /?/ (a voiced palatal fricative, written y). But for most speakers in Spain and the Americas, these two phonemes have been merged in the phoneme /?/. This merger results in the words calló ("silenced") and cayó ("fell") being pronounced the same, whereas they remain distinct in dialects that have not undergone the merger. The use of the merged phoneme is called "yeísmo".
In Spain, the distinction is preserved in some rural areas and smaller cities of the north, while in South America the contrast is characteristic of bilingual areas where Quechua languages and other indigenous languages that have the /?/ sound in their inventories are spoken (this is the case of inland Peru and Bolivia), and in Paraguay.
The phoneme /?/ can be pronounced in a variety of ways, depending on the dialect. In most of the area where yeísmo is present, the merged phoneme /? ~ ?/ is pronounced as the fricative or approximant [?] or as the glide [j], and also, in word-initial positions, glide [j], affricates [??] and [d?] (the latter is also used in other positions as variants of [?] or [j]). In the area around the Río de la Plata (Argentina, Uruguay), this phoneme is pronounced as a palatoalveolar sibilant fricative, either as voiced [?] or, especially by young speakers, as voiceless [?].
Variants of /s/
One of the most distinctive features of the Spanish variants is the pronunciation of /s/ when it is not aspirated to [h] or elided. In northern and central Spain, and in Paisa Region, Colombia, as well as some other isolated dialects (e.g. some inland areas of Peru and of Bolivia), the sibilant realization of /s/ is an apico-alveolar retracted fricative [s?], a sound transitional between laminodental [s] and palatal [?]. However, in most of Andalusia, in a few other areas in southern Spain, and in most of Hispanic America it is rather pronounced as a lamino-alveolar or dental realization. /s/ is allophonized as [z] or [z?] before voiced consonants when it is not aspirated to [h] or elided; [z?] is a sound transitional between [z] and [?].
Debuccalization of coda /s/
In much of Hispanic America--especially in the Caribbean and in coastal and lowland areas of Central and South America--and in the southern half of Spain, syllable-final /s/ is either pronounced as a voiceless glottal fricative, [h]) (debuccalization, also frequently called "aspiration"), or not pronounced at all. In some varieties of Hispanic American Spanish (notably Honduran Spanish) this may also occur intervocalically within an individual word, as with nosotros. which may be pronounced as [nohotroh]. In southeastern Spain (eastern Andalusia, Murcia and part of La Mancha), the distinction between syllables with a now-silent s and those originally without s is preserved by pronouncing the syllables ending in s with open vowels (that is, the open/closed syllable contrast has been turned into a tense/lax vowel contrast); this typically affects the vowels /a/, /e/ and /o/, but in some areas even /i/ and /u/ are affected. For instance, todos los cisnes son blancos ("all the swans are white"), can be pronounced ['t?o?ðo?? lo?? '?:i?n:e?? 'sõ?? 'blã?ko??], or even ['t??ð? l? '?:???n:? 's??? 'blã?k?] (Standard Peninsular Spanish: ['to?ðoz? los? '?iz?ne?'s?:õ?m 'blã?kos?], Latin American Spanish: ['to?ðoz lo?'s:izne?'s:õ?m 'blã?kos]). This open-closed vowel contrast is sometimes reinforced by vowel harmony. For those areas of southeastern Spain where the deletion of final /s/ is complete, and where the distinction between singular and plural of nouns depends entirely on vowel quality, the case has been made to claim that a set of phonemic splits has occurred, resulting in a system with eight vowel phonemes in place of the standard five.
Vowel reduction
Although the vowels of Spanish are relatively stable from one dialect to another, the phenomenon of vowel reduction--devoicing or even loss--of unstressed vowels in contact with voiceless consonants, especially /s/, can be observed in the speech of central Mexico (including Mexico City). For example, it can be the case that the words pesos ("pesos [money]"), pesas ("weights"), and peces ("fish [pl.]") sound nearly the same, as ['pe?ss?] (with the second [s] much like a syllabic consonant). One may hear pues ("well then") pronounced [p?s]. Some efforts to explain this vowel reduction link it to the strong influence of Nahuatl and other Native American languages in Mexican Spanish.
Pronunciation of j
In the 16th century, as the Spanish colonization of the Americas was beginning, the phoneme now represented by the letter j had begun to change its place of articulation from palato-alveolar [?] to palatal [ç] and to velar [x], like German ch in Bach (see History of Spanish and Old Spanish language). In southern Spanish dialects and in those Hispanic American dialects strongly influenced by southern settlers (e.g. Caribbean Spanish), rather than the velar fricative [x], the result was a softer glottal sound [h], like English h in hope. Glottal [h] is nowadays the standard pronunciation for j in Caribbean dialects (Cuban, Dominican, and Puerto Rican) as well as in mainland Venezuela, much of Central America, southern Mexico, the Canary Islands and western Andalusia in Spain; in the rest of the country, [x] alternates with a "raspy" uvular fricative [?], sometimes accompanied by uvular vibration. In the rest of the Americas, the velar fricative [x] is prevalent. In Chile, [x] becomes the more frontal [ç] (like German ch in ich) when it precedes palatal vowels [i], [e]: gente ['ç??n?t?e], jinete [çi'ne?t?e]; in other phonological environments it is pronounced either [h] or [x].
Word-final -n
In standard European Spanish, as well as in many dialects in the Americas (e.g. standard Argentine or Rioplatense, Colombian, and Mexican), word-final /n/ is, by default (i.e. when followed by a pause or by an initial vowel in the following word), alveolar, like English [n] in pen. When followed by a consonant, it assimilates to that consonant's place of articulation, becoming dental, interdental, palatal, or velar. In some dialects, however, word-final /n/ without a following consonant is pronounced as a velar nasal [?] (like the -ng of English long), and may produce vowel nasalization. In these dialects, words such as pan ('bread') and bien ('well') may sound like pang and byeng to English-speakers. Because of this pronunciation, loanwords based on English words with final -ng sound similar to their original pronunciation: mitin ('political rally', from Eng. meeting) pronounced as ['miti?]; ranking as ['ra?ki?]; marketing as ['ma?keti?]; and pudín (Eng. pudding) as [pu'ði?]. Velar -n is common in many parts of Spain (Galicia, León, Asturias, Murcia, Extremadura, Andalusia, and Canary Islands). In the Americas, velar -n is prevalent in all Caribbean dialects, Central American dialects, the coastal areas of Colombia, Venezuela, much of Ecuador, Peru, and northern Chile. Loss of final -n with strong nasalization of the preceding vowel is not infrequent in all those dialects where velar -n exists. In much of Ecuador, Peru, Venezuela (except for the Andean region) and Dominican Spanish, any pre-consonantal /n/ or /m/ can be realized [?]; thus, a word like ambientación can be pronounced [ã?bj???t?a'sjõ??].
R sounds
All varieties of Spanish distinguish between a "single-R" and a "double-R" phoneme. The single-R phoneme corresponds to the letter r written once (except when word-initial or following l, n, or s) and is pronounced as [?], an alveolar flap--like American English tt in better--in virtually all dialects. The single-R/double-R contrast is neutralized in syllable-final position, and in some dialects these phonemes also lose their contrast with /l/, so a word such as artesanía may sound like altesanía. This neutralization or "leveling" of /?/ and /l/ is frequent in dialects of southern Spain, the Caribbean, Venezuela and coastal Colombia.
The double-R phoneme is spelled rr between vowels (as in carro 'car') and r word-initially (e.g. rey 'king', ropa 'clothes') or following l, n, or s (e.g. alrededor 'around', enriquecer 'enrich', enrollar 'roll up', honra 'honor', Israel 'Israel'). In most varieties it is pronounced as an alveolar trill [r], and that is considered the prestige pronunciation. Two notable variants occur additionally: one sibilant and the other velar or uvular. The trill is also found in lexical derivations (morpheme-initial positions), and prefixation with sub and ab: abrogado [a?.ro'?a(ð)o] (also pronounced [a?.ro'?a(ð)o], [a?.r:o'?a(ð)o] or [a?.r:o'?a(ð)o]), 'abrogated', subrayar [su?.ra'jar] (also pronounced [su?.ra'jar], [su?.ra'jar] or [su?.ra'jar]), 'to stress'. The same goes for compound word ciudadrealeño (from Ciudad Real) However, after vowels, the initial r of the root becomes rr in prefixed or compound words: prorrogar, infrarrojo, autorretrato, puertorriqueño, Monterrey. In syllable-final position, inside a word, the tap is more frequent, but the trill can also occur (especially in emphatic or oratorical style) with no semantic difference, especially before l, m, n, or s--thus arma ('weapon') may be either ['a?ma] (tap) or ['arma] (trill), perla ('pearl') may be either ['pe??la] or ['pe?rla], invierno ('winter') may be [?m'bje??no] or [?m'bje?rno], verso ("verse") may be ['be??so] or ['be?rso], and verde ("green") ['be??ðe] or ['be?rðe]. In word-final position the rhotic will usually be: either a trill or a tap when followed by a consonant or a pause, as in amo[r ~ ?] paterno 'paternal love'), amo[r ~ ?]; or a tap when the followed by a vowel-initial word, as in amo[?] eterno 'eternal love').
The pronunciation of the double-R phoneme as a voiced strident (or sibilant) apical fricative is common in New Mexico, Guatemala, and Costa Rica; in highland areas of Colombia, Ecuador, Bolivia, and Chile; in western and northern Argentina, and in Paraguay. Some linguists have attempted to explain the assibilated rr as a result of influence from Native American languages, and it is true that in the Andean regions mentioned an important part of the population is bilingual in Spanish and one or another indigenous language. Nonetheless, other researchers have pointed out that sibilant rr in the Americas may not be an autonomous innovation, but rather a pronunciation that originated in some northern Spanish dialects and then was exported to the Americas. Spanish dialects spoken in the Basque Country, Navarre, La Rioja, and northern Aragon (regions that contributed substantially to Spanish-American colonization) show the fricative or postalveolar variant for rr (especially for the word-initial rr sound, as in Roma or rey). In Andean regions, the alveolar trill is realized as an alveolar approximant [?] or even as a voiced apico-alveolar /z/, and it is quite common in inland Ecuador, Peru, most of Bolivia and in parts of northern Argentina and Paraguay. The alveolar approximant realization is particularly associated with a substrate from Native American languages. In Ecuador and Bolivia, /r/ and /?/ are assibilated to [r?] and [??], respectively; also particularly associated with a substrate from Native American languages.
The other major variant for the rr phoneme--common in Puerto Rico and the Dominican Republic--is articulated at the back of the mouth, either as a glottal [h] followed by a voiceless apical trill or, especially in Puerto Rico, with a posterior articulation that ranges variously "from a velar [x] to a uvular trill [?]." Canfield transcribes the sound as uppercase [R] with a ring below, [??], evidently for a voiceless trill. These realizations for rr maintain their contrast with the phoneme /x/, as the latter tends to be realized as a soft glottal [h]: compare Ramón [xa'mõ??] ~ [??a'mõ??] ("Raymond") with jamón [ha'mõ??] ("ham").
In Puerto Rico, syllable-final /r/ can be realized as [?] (probably an influence of American English), aside from [?], [r], and [l], so that verso ("verse") becomes ['be??so], alongside ['be??so], ['be?rso], or ['be?lso]; invierno ("winter") becomes [?m'bje??no], alongside [?m'bje??no], [?m'bje?rno], or [im'bje?lno]; and parlamento (parliament) becomes [pa?la'mento], aside from [pa?la'mento], [parla'mento], or [pala'mento]. In word-final position, the realization of /r/ depends on whether it is followed by a consonant-initial word or a pause, on one hand, or by a vowel-initial word on the other:
- Before a consonant or pause: a trill, a tap, an approximant, the lateral [l], or elided, as in amo[r ~ ? ~ ? ~ l] paterno "paternal love", or amor [a'mo]).
- Before a vowel: a tap, an approximant, or the lateral [l], as in amo[?~ ? ~ l] eterno "eternal love").
The same situation happens in Belize and Archipelago of San Andrés, Providencia and Santa Catalina, an influence of British English.
In Paraguay, syllable-final /r/ is pronounced as [?] before l or s, influenced by a substrate from Native American languages.
In Chile, as in Andalusia, [r] or [?] in the sequence /rn/ or /?n/ is sometimes assimilated to [n:] in lower-class speakers, and sometimes in educated speakers. Thus, jornada ([xor'n?aða] or [hor'n?aða]) 'workday' may be pronounced [xo'n?:a:] or [ho'n?:a:].
Pronunciation of x
The letter x usually represents the phoneme sequence /ks/. An exception to the correspondence of x with /ks/ is the pronunciation of the x in some place names, especially in Mexico, such as Oaxaca and the name México itself, reflecting an older spelling (see "Name of Mexico"). Some personal names, such as Javier, Jiménez, Rojas, etc., also are occasionally spelled with X: Xavier, Ximénez, Roxas, etc. and the pronunciation of x is either [x] or [h]. A small number of words in Mexican Spanish retain the historical /?/ pronunciation, e.g. Mexica. In emphatic speech, x is pronounced [ks], but in casual speech the /k/, being syllable-final, can weaken to a voiced fricative [?] or disappear altogether, especially when the x is followed by a consonant; the remaining /s/ will be aspirated to [h] or elided in dialects with this trait. The tendency to delete the /k/ element is generally stronger in Spain than in the Spanish of Hispanic America. When the x is followed by ce or ci (e.g. excelente, excitar), in dialects using seseo (see above), the /s/ of the /ks/ sequence merges with the /s/ that corresponds to the letter c, so excelente may be pronounced either [e?kse'l??nte], [e??se'l??nte] or [e??s:e'l??n?t?e]. But in those areas of northern and central Spain that distinguish between the phonemes /s/ and /?/, when the /s/ is not aspirated to [h] or elided, excelente is pronounced [e?s??e'l??n?t?e] or [e??s??e'l??n?t?e]. If the x is followed by consonants, it may be pronounced /ks/ or may merge as /s/ (e.g. exclamar [e?kskla'mar], [e?(?)skla'mar]); the latter pronunciation is true, because syllable-final position cannot be more than one consonant in informal sociolects, and in many cases, it is allowed even in the formal sociolects. Dialects that practice debuccalization of syllable-final /s/ (see above) treat the /s/ of a syllable-final (letter) x in the same way, so that exclamar is pronounced [e??k:la'mar/?/l] ~ [??k:la'ma(r)].
Adoption of the affricates tz and tl
Mexican Spanish and some other Hispanic American dialects have adopted from the native languages the voiceless alveolar affricate [t?s] and the cluster [tl] (originally [t?]) represented by the respective digraphs <tz> and <tl>, as in the names Atzcapotzalco and Tlaxcala. In these dialects, even words of Greek and Latin origin with <tl>, such as Atlántico and atleta, are pronounced with the affricate: [a'tlãn?t?iko?], [a'tle?t?a] (compare [að'lãn?t?iko?], [að'le?t?a] in Spain and other dialects in Hispanic America). The [t?s] sound also occurs in European Spanish in loanwords of Basque origin (but only learned loanwords, not those inherited from Roman times), as in abertzale. In colloquial Castilian it may be replaced by /t?/ or /?/. In Bolivian, Paraguayan , and Coastal Peruvian Spanish, [t?s] also occurs in loanwords of Japanese origin.
Other loaned phonetics
Spanish has a fricative [?] for loanwords of origins from native languages in Mexican Spanish, loanwords of French, German and English origin in Chilean Spanish, loanwords of Italian, Galician, French, German and English origin in Rioplatense Spanish and Venezuelan Spanish, Chinese loanwords in Coastal Peruvian Spanish, Japanese loanwords in Bolivian Spanish, Paraguayan Spanish, Coastal Peruvian Spanish, Basque loanwords in Castilian Spanish (but only learned loanwords, not those inherited from Roman times), and English loanwords in Puerto Rican Spanish and all dialects.
Pronunciation of ch
The Spanish digraph ch is pronounced [t??]. /t??/ is pronounced as a fricative [?] in some Andalusian dialects, New Mexican Spanish, northern Mexican Spanish, informal Panamanian Spanish, and informal Chilean Spanish. This type of pronunciation is viewed as undesirable in Chilean, while this is also pronounced by educated speakers of Panamanian dialect. This is pronounced as alveolar affricate, [t?s] in Madrid and among upper and middle class Chilean speakers.
Open-mid vowels
In some dialects of southeastern Spain (Murcia, eastern Andalusia and a few adjoining areas) where the weakening of final /s/ leads to its disappearance, the "silent" /s/ continues to have an effect on the preceding vowel, opening the mid vowels /e/ and /o/ to [?] and [?] respectively, and fronting the low vowel /a/ toward [æ]. Thus the singular/plural distinction in nouns and adjectives is maintained by means of the vowel quality:
- libro ['li ??o] 'book', but libros ['li ???] 'books'.
- libre ['li ??e? ] 'free, singular ', but libres ['li ???] 'free, plural'.
- libra ['li ??a] 'pound', but libras ['li ??æ] 'pounds'.
Furthermore, this opening of final mid vowels can affect other vowels earlier in the word, as an instance of vowel harmony:
- lobo ['lo ?o] 'wolf', but lobos ['l? ??] 'wolves'.
(In the remaining dialects, the mid vowels have nondistinctive open and closed allophones determined by the shape of the syllable or by contact with neighboring phonemes. See Spanish phonology.)
Judaeo-Spanish
Judaeo-Spanish (often called Ladino) refers to the Romance dialects spoken by Jews whose ancestors were expelled from Spain near the end of the 15th century.
These dialects have important phonological differences compared to varieties of Spanish proper; for example, they have preserved the voiced/voiceless distinction among sibilants as they were in Old Spanish. For this reason, the letter s, when written single between vowels, corresponds to a voiced [z]--e.g. rosa ['ro?za] ("rose"). Where s is not between vowels and is not followed by a voiced consonant, or when it is written double, it corresponds to voiceless [s]--thus assentarse [as??n'tarse?] ("to sit down"). And due to a phonemic neutralization similar to the seseo of other dialects, the Old Spanish voiced z [dz] and the voiceless ç [ts] have merged, respectively, with /z/ and /s/--while maintaining the voicing contrast between them. Thus fazer ("to make") has gone from the medieval [fa'dze?r] to [fa'ze?r], and plaça ("town square") has gone from ['platsa] to ['plasa].
A related dialect is Haketia, the Judaeo-Spanish of northern Morocco. This too tended to assimilate with modern Spanish, during the Spanish occupation of the region. Tetuani Ladino was brought to Oran in Algeria.
Grammar
Variation in second-person pronouns and verbs
Spanish is a language with a "T-V distinction" in the second person, meaning that there are different pronouns corresponding to "you" which express different degrees of formality. In most varieties, there are two degrees, namely "formal" and "familiar" (the latter is also called "informal").
For the second person formal, virtually all Spanish dialects of Spain and the Americas use usted and ustedes (singular and plural respectively). But for the second person familiar, there is regional variation--between tú and vos for the singular, and, separately, between vosotros and ustedes for the plural. The use of vos (and its corresponding verb forms) rather than tú is called voseo.
Each of the second-person pronouns has its historically corresponding verb forms, used by most speakers. Most voseo speakers use both the pronoun vos and its historically corresponding verb forms (e.g. vos tenés, "you have"). But some dialects use the pronoun tú with "vos verb forms" (verbal voseo--tú tenés), while others use vos with "tú verb forms" (pronominal voseo--vos tienes).
Second person singular
In most dialects the familiar second person singular pronoun is tú (from Latin t?), and the formal pronoun is usted (usually considered to originate from vuestra merced, meaning "your grace" or, literally, "your mercy"). In a number of regions in the Americas, tú is replaced by another pronoun, vos, and the verb conjugation changes accordingly (see details below). Spanish vos comes from Latin v?s, the second person plural pronoun in Latin.
In any case, there is wide variation as to when each pronoun (formal or familiar) is used. In Spain, tú is familiar (for example, used with friends), and usted is formal (for example, used with older people). In recent times, there has been a noticeable tendency to extend the use of tú even in situations previously reserved for usted. Meanwhile, in several countries (in parts of Middle America, especially, Costa Rica and Colombia), the formal usted is also used to denote a closer personal relationship. Many Colombians and some Chileans, for instance, use usted for a child to address a parent and also for a parent to address a child. Some countries, such as Cuba and the Dominican Republic, prefer the use of tú even in very formal circumstances, and usted is seldom used.
Meanwhile, in other countries, the use of formal rather than familiar second-person pronouns denotes authority. In Peru, for example, senior military officers use tú to speak to their subordinates, but junior officers use only usted to address their superior officers.
Using the familiar tú, especially in contexts where usted was to be expected, is called tuteo. The corresponding verb is tutear (a transitive verb, the direct object being the person addressed with the pronoun). The verb tutear is used even in those dialects whose familiar pronoun is vos and means "to treat with the familiar second-person pronoun".
Pronominal voseo, the use of the pronoun vos instead of tú, is the prevalent form of the familiar second person singular pronoun in Guatemala, El Salvador, Honduras, Nicaragua, Costa Rica, Paraguay, Argentina and Uruguay. In those countries, it is used by many to address others in all kinds of contexts, often regardless of social status or age, including by cultured/educated speakers and writers, in television, advertisements, and even in translations from other languages. In Guatemala and Uruguay, vos and tú are used concurrently, but vos is much more common. Both pronouns use the verb forms normally associated with vos (vos querés / tú querés, "you want").
The name Rioplatense is applied to the dialect of Spanish spoken around the mouth of the Río de la Plata and the lower course of the Paraná River, where vos, not tú, is invariably used, with the vos verb forms (vos tenés). The area comprises the most populous part of Argentina (the provinces of Buenos Aires and Santa Fe) as well as an important part of Uruguay, including Montevideo, the capital.
In Ecuador, vos is the most prominent form throughout the Sierra region of the country, though it does coexist with usted and the lesser-used tú. In this region, vos is regarded as the conversational norm, but it is not used in public discourse or the mass media. The choice of pronoun to be used depends on the participants' likeness in age and/or social status. Based on these factors, speakers can assess themselves as being equal, superior, or inferior to the addressee, and the choice of pronoun is made on this basis, sometimes resulting in a three-tiered system. Ecuadorians of the Highlands thus generally use vos among familiarized equals or by superiors (in both social status and age) to inferiors; tú among unfamiliarized equals, or by a superior in age but inferior in social status; and usted by both familiarized and unfamiliarized inferiors, or by a superior in social status but inferior in age. In the more-populated coastal region, the form tú is used in most situations, usted being used only for unfamiliar and/or superior addressees.
Vos can be heard throughout most of Chile, Bolivia, and a small part of Peru as well, but in these places it is regarded as substandard. It is also used as the conversational norm in the Paisa Region and the southwest region of Colombia, in Zulia State (Venezuela), in Honduras, El Salvador, Costa Rica, Guatemala, and the state of Chiapas in Mexico.
In Chile, even though tú is the prestige pronoun among educated speakers, the use of "verbal voseo", i.e. «tú + verb conjugation of vos» (e.g. "tú podís") is widespread. On the other hand, "pronominal voseo", the use of the pronoun vos--pronounced with aspiration of the final -s--is used derisively in informal speech between close friends as playful banter (usually among men) or, depending on the tone of voice, as an offensive comment.
In Colombia, the choice of second person singular varies with location. In most of inland Colombia (especially the Andean region), usted is the pronoun of choice for all situations, even in speaking between friends or family; but in large cities (especially Bogotá), the use of tú is becoming more accepted in informal situations, especially between young interlocutors of opposite sexes and among young women. In Valle del Cauca (Cali), Antioquia (Medellín) and the Pacific coast, the pronouns used are vos and usted. On the Caribbean coast (mainly Barranquilla and Cartagena), tú is used for practically all informal situations and many formal situations as well, usted being reserved for the most formal environments. A peculiarity occurs in Boyacá and among older speakers in Bogotá: usted is replaced by sumercé for formal situations (it is relatively easy to identify a Boyacense by his/her use of this pronoun). Sumercé comes from su merced ("your mercy").
In parts of Spain, a child used to use not tú but usted to address a parent. Today, however, this usage is unusual. Among the factors for the ongoing replacement of usted by tú are the new social relevance of youth and the reduction of social differences. In particular, it has been attributed to the egalitarianism of the right-wing Falange party. By contrast, Spanish leftists of the early 20th century would address their comrades as usted as a show of respect and workers' dignity.
According to Joan Coromines, by the 16th century, the use of vos (as a second person singular pronoun) had been reduced to rural areas of Spain, which were a source of many emigrants to the New World, and so vos became the unmarked form in many areas of Hispanic America.
A slightly different explanation is that in Spain, even if vos (as a singular) originally denoted the high social status of those who were addressed as such (monarchs, nobility, etc.), the people never used the pronoun themselves since there were few or no people above them in society. Those who used vos were people of the lower classes and peasants. When the waves of Spanish immigrants arrived to populate the New World, they primarily came from these lower classes. In the New World, wanting to raise their social status from what it was in Spain, they demanded to be addressed as vos.
Through the widespread use of vos in the Americas, the pronoun was transformed into an indicator of low status not only for the addresser but also for the addressee. Conversely, in Spain, vos is now considered a highly exalted archaism virtually confined to liturgy.
Speakers of Ladino still use vos as it was used in the Middle Ages, to address people higher on the social ladder. The pronoun usted had not been introduced to this dialect of Spanish when the Jews were expelled from Spain in 1492 so vos is still used in Ladino much as usted is used in modern Spanish.
A variant of usted, vusted, can be heard in Andean regions of South America. Other, less frequent forms analogous to usted are vuecencia (short for "vuestra excelencia"), and usía (from "vuestra señoría").
There is a traditional assumption that Chilean and River Plate voseo verb forms are derived from those corresponding to vosotros. This assumption, however, has been challenged, in an article by Baquero & Westphal (2014)--in the theoretical framework of classical generative phonology--as synchronically inadequate, on the grounds that it requires at least six different rules, including three monophthongization processes that lack phonological motivation. Alternatively, the article argues that the Chilean and River Plate voseo verb forms are synchronically derived from underlying representations that coincide with those corresponding to the non-honorific second person singular tú. The proposed theory requires the use of only one special rule in the case of Chilean voseo. This rule--along with other rules that are independently justified in the language--makes it possible to derive synchronically all Chilean and River Plate voseo verb forms in a straightforward manner. The article additionally solves the problem posed by the alternate verb forms of Chilean voseo such as the future indicative (e.g. vay a bailar "you will dance"), the present indicative forms of haber (habih and hai "you have"), and the present indicative of ser (soi, eríh and erei "you are"), without resorting to any ad hoc rules.
Second person plural
In Standard European Spanish the plural of tú is vosotros and the plural of usted is ustedes. In Hispanic America vosotros is not used, and the plural of both tú and usted is ustedes. This means that speaking to a group of friends a Spaniard will use vosotros, while a Hispanic American Spanish-speaker will use ustedes. Although ustedes is semantically a second-person form, it is treated grammatically as a third-person plural form because it originates from the term vuestras mercedes ("your [pl.] graces," sing. vuestra merced).
The only vestiges of vosotros in the Americas are boso/bosonan in Papiamento and the use of vuestro/a in place of sus (de ustedes) as second person plural possessive in the Cusco region of Peru.
In very formal contexts, however, the vosotros conjugation can still be found. An example is the Mexican national anthem, which contains such forms as apretad and empapad.
The plural of the Colombian (Cundi-Boyacense Plateau) sumercé is sumercés/susmercedes, from sus mercedes ("your mercies").
In some parts of Andalusia (the lands around the Guadalquivir river and western Andalusia), the usage is what is called ustedes-vosotros: the pronoun ustedes is combined with the verb forms for vosotros. However, this sounds extremely colloquial and most Andalusians prefer to use each pronoun with its correct form.
In Ladino, vosotros is still the only second person plural pronoun, since ustedes does not exist.
Second-person verb forms
Each second-person pronoun has its historically corresponding verb forms. The formal usted and ustedes, although semantically second person, take verb forms identical with those of the third person, singular and plural respectively, since they are derived from the third-person expressions vuestra merced and vuestras mercedes ("your grace", "your graces"). The forms associated with the singular vos can generally be derived from those for the plural vosotros by deleting the palatal semivowel of the ending (vosotros habláis > vos hablás, "you speak"; vosotros coméis > vos comés, "you eat").
General statements about the use of voseo in different localities should be qualified by the note that individual speakers may be inconsistent in their usage, and that isoglosses rarely coincide with national borders. That said, a few assertions can be made:
- "Full" voseo (involving both pronoun and verb--vos comés, "you eat") is characteristic of two zones: that of Argentina, Paraguay and Uruguay and that of Central America and the Mexican state of Chiapas.
- "Full" voseo coexists with the use of tú and its verb forms (e.g. tú comes) in Colombia and Ecuador, and in parts of Colombia also with usted (with its standard verb forms) as a familiar form.
- In Chile there is coexistence of three usages:
- tú and its verb forms (tú comes);
- "full" voseo with uniquely Chilean voseo verb endings (-ái, -ís, and -ís respectively for -ar, -er, and -ir verbs: vos hablái--"you speak", vos comís--"you eat", vos vivís--"you live"); and
- verbal voseo with the Chilean verb endings (tú hablái, tú comís, etc.).
- "Full" voseo coexists with verbal voseo (tú comés) in Uruguay.
- In Venezuela's Zulia State and parts of the state of Falcón there is no deletion of the palatal semivowel, so you have "vos coméis", "vos habláis", "vos seáis". In Trujillo State, the "voseo" is like that of Argentina, with the exception of the imperative mood, which is like that of the standard tú.
- Voseo is absent from the Spanish of Spain, and from most of Mexico, Peru, and the islands of the Caribbean.
As for the second person familiar plural, it can be said that northern and central Spain use vosotros and its verb forms (vosotros habláis, "you [pl.] speak"), while the rest of the Spanish-speaking world merges the familiar and formal in ustedes (ustedes hablan). Usage in western Andalusia includes the use of ustedes with the traditional vosotros verb form (ustedes habláis).
In Ladino, the second-person pronouns are quite different from those of Spain and Hispanic America. The forms usted and ustedes had not yet appeared in 1492 when the Jews were expelled from Spain. Speakers of Ladino still use vos as it was used in the Middle Ages (as a singular) to address people higher on the social ladder. And vosotros is the only second person plural pronoun. In Ladino the formal singular for "you speak" is vos avláis (pronounced [a'vla?], and the same verb form serves for the plural, both formal and familiar: vosotros avláis ([vo'zotros a'vla?]). The subjunctive "that you lose" (formal singular) is que vos pedráis ([ke vos pe'dra?]), while the plural (both formal and familiar) is que vosotros pedráis ([ke vo'zotros pe'dra?]). The formal singular imperative ("come") is venid or vení, and the same form serves as the plural imperative, both formal and familiar.
Verb tenses for past events
In a broad sense, when expressing an action viewed as finished in the past, speakers (and writers) in most of Spain use the perfect tense--e.g. he llegado "I have arrived"--more often than their Hispanic American counterparts, while Spanish-speakers in the Americas more often use the preterite (llegué "I arrived").
The perfect is also called the "present perfect" and, in Spanish, pasado perfecto or pretérito perfecto compuesto. It is described as a "compound" tense (compuesto in Spanish) because it is formed with the auxiliary verb haber plus a main verb.
The preterite, also called the "simple past" and, in Spanish, pretérito indefinido or pretérito perfecto simple, is considered a "simple" tense because it is formed of a single word: the verb stem with an inflectional ending for person, number, etc.
The choice between preterite and perfect, according to prescriptive grammars from both Spain and Hispanic America, is based on the psychological time frame--whether expressed or merely implied--in which the past action is embedded. If that time frame includes the present moment (i.e. if the speaker views the past action as somehow related to the moment of speaking), then the recommended tense is the perfect (he llegado). But if the time frame does not include the present--if the speaker views the action as only in the past, with little or no relation to the moment of speaking--then the recommended tense is the preterite (llegué). This is also the real spontaneous usage in most of Spain.
Following this criterion, an explicit time frame such as hoy "today" or este año "this year" includes the present and thus dictates the compound tense: Este año he cantado "I have sung this year". Conversely, a time frame such as ayer "yesterday" or la semana pasada "last week" does not include the present and therefore calls for the preterite: La semana pasada canté "I sang last week".
However, in most of Hispanic America, and in the Canary Islands, the preterite is used for all actions viewed as completed in the past. It tends to be used in the same way in those parts of Spain where the local languages and vernaculars do not have compound tenses, that is, the Galician-speaking area and the neighbouring Astur-Leonese-speaking area.
In most of Spain, the compound tense is preferred in most cases when the action described is close to the present moment:
- He viajado a los Estados Unidos. "I have (just) traveled to the USA."
- Cuando he llegado, la he visto. "When I arrived, I saw her."
- ¿Qué ha pasado? "What (has) happened?"
Prescriptive norms would rule out the compound tense in a cuando-clause, as in the second example above.
Meanwhile, in Galicia, León, Asturias, Canary Islands, and Hispanic America, speakers follow the opposite tendency, using the simple past tense in most cases, even if the action takes place at some time close to the present:
- Ya viajé a los Estados Unidos. "I (have already) traveled to the USA."
- Cuando llegué, la vi. "When I arrived, I saw her."
- ¿Qué pasó? "What happened?"
For some speakers of Hispanic American Spanish, the compound tense can sound affected, bookish, or foreign.
In Hispanic America one could say He viajado a España varias veces, "I have traveled to Spain several times", to express a repeated action, as in English. But to say El año pasado he viajado a España would sound ungrammatical (as it would also be in English to say "Last year, I have traveled to Spain", as "last year" implies that the relevant time period does not include the present). In Spain, speakers use the compound tense when the period of time considered has not ended, as in He comprado un coche este año "I have bought a car this year". Meanwhile, a Hispanic American Spanish-speaker is more likely to say Compré un carro este año meaning "I bought a car this year".
Vocabulary
Different regional varieties of Spanish vary in terms of vocabulary as well. This includes both words that exist only in certain varieties (especially words borrowed from indigenous languages of the Americas), and words that are used differently in different areas. Among words borrowed from indigenous languages are many names for food, plants and animals, clothes, and household object, such as the following items of Mexican Spanish vocabulary borrowed from Nahuatl.
In addition to loan words, there are a number of Spanish words that have developed distinct senses in different regional dialects. That is, for certain words a distinct meaning, either in addition to the standard meaning or in place of it, exists in some varieties of Spanish.
Mutual comprehension
The different dialects and accents do not block cross-understanding among the educated. Meanwhile, the basilects have diverged more. The unity of the language is reflected in the fact that early imported sound films were dubbed into one version for the entire Spanish-speaking market. Currently, films not originally in Spanish (usually Hollywood productions) are dubbed separately into two accents: one for Spain and one for Hispanic American (using a Mexican or Puerto Rican accent without regionalisms). Some high-budget productions, however, such as the Harry Potter film series, have had dubs in three or more of the major accents. On the other hand, productions from another Spanish-language country are seldom dubbed. Exceptionally, the made-in-Spain animated features Dogtanian and the Three Muskehounds and The World of David the Gnome, as well as TV serials from the Southern Cone such as Karkú (Chile) and Lalola (Argentina), have had a Mexican dub. The popularity of telenovelas and music familiarizes the speakers with other accents of Spanish.
Prescription and a common cultural and literary tradition, among other factors, have contributed to the formation of a loosely defined register which can be termed Standard Spanish (or "Neutral Spanish"), which is the preferred form in formal settings, and is considered indispensable in academic and literary writing, the media, etc. This standard tends to disregard local grammatical, phonetic and lexical peculiarities, and draws certain extra features from the commonly acknowledged canon, preserving (for example) certain verb tenses considered "bookish" or archaic in most other dialects.
See also
Cants and argots
- Bron of migrant merchants and artisans of Asturias and León
- Caló language of Gitanos
- Caló of Chicanos
- Cheli of Madrid
- Gacería of Cantalejo, Spain
- Germanía of Spanish Golden Century criminals
- Lunfardo of Porteño Spanish
Mixes with other languages
- Spanish-based creole languages
- Annobonese language of Annobón Province and Bioko, Equatorial Guinea
- Belgranodeutsch of Buenos Aires
- Castrapo of Galicia
- Chavacano of the Philippines
- Cocoliche of Buenos Aires
- Frespañol of French-Spanish contact
- Judaeo-Spanish, also known as Ladino, the language of the Sephardic Jews
- Llanito of Gibraltar
- Palenquero of Colombia
- Papiamento of Aruba, Curaçao and Bonaire
- Pichinglis of Bioko, Equatorial Guinea
- Portuñol of the Brazilian border
- Spanglish of the United States of America
Other
- History of the Spanish language
- Spanish phonology
- Castilian Spanish
- Central American Spanish
- South American Spanish
- Equatoguinean Spanish
- Philippine Spanish
Notes
Bibliography
Further reading
- Alonso Zamora Vicente, Dialectología Española (Madrid: Editorial Gredos, 1960) is highly detailed.
External links
- Isogloss maps of phonetic variants in the Iberian Peninsula
- Map of Spanish dialects in the Iberian Peninsula
- Costa Rican Spanish Dictionary
- Spanish learning site with Argentinian speakers
- Hispanic American Dictionary with variants for every country
- Spanish dialects, pronunciation maps
Source of the article : Wikipedia